Issue 01: The Anarchists Guide to Critical Thinking

    The original pamphlet with more illustrations can be found on the internet archive.
     

    Peer Review Issue 01 Transcript

    What is critical
    thinking...

    Some writers and
    philosophers have approach defining it broad strokes: Robert Ennis,
    who spent six decades writing about the topic, claimed that critical
    thinking is simply “reasonable reflective thinking that is focused
    on deciding what to believe or do.” (1991, p. 8). Similarly, Sharon
    Bailin and her colleagues identified only three characteristics that
    make thinking critical: (1) it is done to determine what to believe
    about something, (2) the thinker is trying to meet some standards of
    adequacy in their thinking; and (3) the thinker does meet those
    standards to an appropriate degree (1999).

    Others have focused
    more specifically on critical thinking as applied to argumentation.
    Mark Battersby, for example, defines it as “the ability and
    inclination to assess claims and arguments” (2016, p. 7), and
    stresses the importance of evaluating evidence to expose false
    claims. Regardless of whether the definition is generic or specific,
    though, most writers agree that critical thinking is a habit that
    requires practice to master.

    ..and why should
    you

    give a fuck?

    Far from a bourgeois
    ideology, critical thinking is a necessary tool for anarchists.
    Anarchism demands that individuals be able to think accurately and
    effectively. From being able to spot exploitative power structures to
    understanding the minutiae of alternative economic theories,
    anarchism is far more than just tossing pipe bombs at cop cars. Even
    the most aware anarchist is in danger of falling for misinformation,
    conspiracy theories, and cults of personality— and before you think
    you're immune, remember that you have identical brain structures to
    the people who fall for it all the time. To avoid those traps,
    anarchists need to be able to think for themselves. When done right,
    critical thinking is a necessary step in the path to liberated,
    individual thinking.

    Here’s the plan

    There’s a
    longstanding debate about whether critical thinking skills are
    generalizable (in that there is a single skillset that applies to all
    areas of inquiry) or if it’s domain-specific (in that each
    discipline—math, science, history, philosophy, etc.—has its own
    set of critical thinking skills). I’m choosing to split the
    difference. In Part One, we'll address two generalizable
    skills: first, we'll discuss evidence gathering and assessment, and
    second, we’ll talk about heuristics, biases, and fallacies. In Part
    Two
    , I'll present a guide to critical thinking specifically
    designed for anarchists, based on Daniel Willingham’s 2019 paper
    “How to Teach Critical Thinking.” Willingham outlines four steps
    that should be taken when teaching critical thinking about any topic:
    first, identify what “critical thinking” means in that domain;
    second, identify the knowledge that is necessary for each
    understanding of critical thinking, third, create a sequence in which
    that knowledge should be learned; and fourth, revisit and relearn.
    With that, let’s get started.

    PART ONE GENERALIZABLE SKILLS

    1. EVIDENCE

    When assessing any proposition, argument, or problem, a good thing
    to ask is: how good is the evidence? Every argument requires
    evidence: if someone were to claim that leprechauns are real, we
    shouldn’t take their claims at face value. Rather, we should ask
    for the proof. After that, we should assess if the evidence they
    provide is adequate.

    In his book Is That a Fact? Mark Battersby divides
    the assessment process into two steps. First, ask if the evidence
    supports the determination. He uses the example of a letter to the
    editor published in 7ime, in which the author claims that her
    “85-year-old mother powerwalks two miles each day, drives her car
    (safely), climbs stairs, does crosswords, reads the daily paper and
    could probably beat [your columnist] at almost anything.” Thus, so
    the writer believes, people in this era must be “living to a
    healthy and ripe old age” (2016, p. 14). As Battersby points out,
    however, just because the writer’s grandmother does these things
    does not mean that all elderly people can do these things—the
    premise does nothing to support the conclusion. Whether or not the
    evidence is true, you should be skeptical of an argument if the
    evidence doesn’t provide any basis for the conclusion. Second, you
    should ask if the evidence is credible. If the above mentioned writer
    had cited a study instead of using her own grandmother as an example,
    you should ask if the sample size was adequate and if the study was
    funded by organizations that may have an interest in promoting its
    conclusion. Or if she had cited a poll conducted among senior
    citizens, you should pay attention to question bias (when the
    phrasing of the poll questions influences the responses) and context
    bias
    (when the context of the poll, such as a preliminary
    introduction by the researchers or the environment of the responder,
    influences the responses) (Battersby, 2016, pp. 29 & 52). Above
    all, you should seek to verify that the information being given to
    you is correct—if the premise is false it could point to an invalid
    or unfounded conclusion.

    Philosophical razors are rules of thumb that can be used to
    metaphorically “shave off” unlikely premises and conclusions. The
    principle of parsimony, for example, holds that explanations
    should be as simple as possible. The most famous formulation of it,
    Occam’s Razor, states that we should only accept the more
    complicated theory if the simpler one cannot explain the event
    (Battersby 2016, p. 23). If you hear a crash, walk upstairs, and see
    a baseball, broken glass, and a group of kids with bats and mitts
    running away, the most likely explanation is that they were playing
    baseball and hit a ball through your window. The theory that aliens
    broke your window and planted the baseball there to frame the
    innocent kids should likely be rejected unless the first explanation
    doesn’t account for some aspect of the situation.

    Similarly, the Sagan Standard, attributed to Carl Sagan in
    his book Broca’s Brain, holds that extraordinary
    claims require extraordinary evidence (1979, p. 73). The claim that a
    new treatment will cure any type of cancer in less than twenty
    minutes requires much more proof than the claim that diet and
    exercise help you lose weight. There are many other philosophical
    razors in existence, but a word of caution: while razors provide good
    bases for ruling out bad arguments, they are not foolproof. Though it
    is overwhelmingly unlikely, perhaps aliens did plant that baseball,
    and that new treatment does cure cancer. So, while they may provide a
    quick-and-easy method of detecting bullshit, they are not infallible.

    2.HEURISTICS BIASES AND FALLACIES

    Heuristics

    Human beings (yourself included) are prone to biases, fallacies,
    and unclear thinking. The work of Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman
    (1974) showed that we tend to rely on quick rules of thumb, called
    heuristics, when making probability judgments. While useful
    when making quick decisions, heuristics are prone to error, as when
    one estimates the probability of a heart attack occurring among a
    certain age group based on how many people they know have had heart
    attacks. Who you know that has had a heart attack has no bearing on
    the actual percentage of people that do, similar to how Battersby’s
    writer assumed that all elderly people are fit and healthy because
    her grandmother is.

    Cass Sunstein (2005) extended Tversky and Kahneman’s work to
    include moral judgments, identifying a list of heuristics that tend
    to guide us when making ethical decisions. He includes, for example,
    the Betrayal Heuristic (in which an offense that includes a betrayal
    of trust is often judged as more immoral than one that does not
    include treachery, such as a close friend stabbing someone in the
    back rather than a known rival) and the Outrage Heuristic (in which
    most people’s judgment of how harsh a punishment should be is
    related to how outraged they are by the offense). Like Tversky and
    Kahneman, he argues that these rules of thumb are prone to giving
    inconsistent or incorrect guidance. One thing to watch out for when
    assessing claims (especially your own claims!) is the underlying
    heuristics that the claimant is using.

    Biases

    A number of other cognitive biases exist, too. Confirmation
    bias
    is the tendency for individuals to unconsciously reject
    information that doesn’t align with their existing beliefs. As
    Margit Oswald and Stefan Grosjean put it, confirmation bias means
    that “information is searched for, interpreted, and remembered in
    such a way that it systematically impedes the possibility that the
    hypothesis can be rejected” (2004, p. 79).

    Framing effects occur when individuals draw different
    conclusions from the same information depending on how that
    information is presented. People are more likely to buy yogurt that
    is advertised as “92% fat free” than they are yogurt that is
    advertised as “8% full fat” even though they are the same
    product. This is because the advertiser is “framing” the first
    with positive language and the second with negative. Problematically,
    this means that “people will choose inconsistently in the sense of
    making different and opposed choices in decision problems that are
    essentially identical” (Kamm, 2007, p. 424)—in other words, how a
    problem is framed will affect what people decide to do about it, even
    though the framing doesn’t actually have anything to do with the
    problem.

    Finally, the illusory truth effect occurs when continued
    repetition of a claim causes it to seem truer than alternatives, even
    if it is false. First identified in a 1977 paper by Lynn Hasher,
    David Goldstein, and Thomas Toppino, they found that their test
    subjects rated a statement as more likely to be true if it was
    repeated to them rather than if they read it once. Importantly, this
    is a prominent reason why propaganda techniques such as the Big
    Lie
    (like Trump’s claim that he won the 2020 election) and the
    firehose of falsehood (like Trump’s constant and endless
    lying) work.

    Fallacies

    Unlike heuristics and biases, which affect how people process
    claims, fallacies are mistakes made in the reasoning behind claims.
    There are hundreds, but below are some of the more common ones:

    • Sweeping generalization — The arguer expands a
      specific case into a general principle that does not always apply.
      For example, claiming “People from that city are always rude”
      takes what may be true of some residents (rudeness) and generalizes
      it to all residents.

    • Begging the question -The arguer leaves out an
      important premise to their argument, usually because they assume
      that it is settled and does not need to be addressed. The claim
      “Killing an innocent person is murder. Murder should be illegal.
      Therefore, abortion should be illegal” leaves out the
      controversial premise “abortion is murder.”

    • Ad hominem – The arguer attacks the character of
      their opponent rather than discussing the issue at hand. For
      example, claiming, “You don’t know anything about climate
      change, you’re too young and inexperienced” avoids engaging with
      the hypothetical young person’s argument by dismissing it based on
      their youth.

    • Straw man – The arguer takes another’s argument,
      extends it to an extreme, and then easily dismisses it. This makes
      it seem as if the arguer succeeded in defeating the original
      argument, but they have only torn down the extended version of it.
      For example, the claim “My opponent wants to reduce carbon
      emissions. Clearly, what he really wants is to ban all cars and shut
      down factories” takes a reasonable argument (reduce carbon
      emissions) and blows it up into an extreme not found in the original
      argument (banning all cars and shutting down factories).

    PART
    TWO

    DOMAIN-SPECIFIC SKILLS

    1. APPLICATIONS

    Now that we’ve covered some general critical thinking skills,
    let us turn to Willingham’s plan to teach domain-specific skills.
    The first step is to identify what critical thinking means for
    anarchists
    . So, what should anarchists be able to do with their
    thinking? While this list is by no means exhaustive, below are some
    ideas.

    Power & Hierarchy

    Key to an anarchist evaluation of the existing social norms is the
    identification of existing hierarchies. After all, one of the core
    axioms of anarchism is that people have no obligation to follow those
    in power (Crowder, 2005). This set of skills may include spotting
    classism/racism/sexism/ableism, identifying structural violence, and
    recognizing cults of personality. Bonus points for assessing the role
    of police, politicians, and judges in perpetuating injustice.

    Economics

    Economic theory is one of the cornerstones of anarchist thought.
    It is not only important to learn and understand anarchist models
    (anarcho-syndicalism, anarcho-communism, etc.) but also to study the
    captalist model that anarchism is working to overthrow. Skills in
    this area include the ability to discover and analyze labor
    exploitation and the basic knowledge required to understand the
    foundations of neoliberalism, communism, and socialism.

    Media

    Media can be both a tool of the state and a source of the truth.
    On the one hand is the corporate media that, as Peter Gelderloos has
    pointed out, exists only to “fatten the wallets of their executives
    and shareholders” and maintain social control (2004). On the other
    is, well, this zine! Skills in this area include identifying
    propaganda, discovering the sources behind specific information and
    narratives, and uncovering media bias in all of its forms (cf.
    Chomsky & Herman, 2002).

    Organization

    What’s the point of being an anarchist if you aren’t willing
    to act? Critical thinking skills in this area include identifying
    methods to engage with activists in other spheres, organizing
    protests, and advocating for alternative systems. Also included in
    this area are skills related to the history and praxis of anarchism,
    especially learning from past and present successes and failures.

    2. CONTENT

    Now that the goals of anarchist thinking have been identified, the
    second step in the process is to gather the knowledge necessary to
    reach those goals.
    Every problem requires the requisite
    background information in order to solve it. The example Willingham
    uses is a historical letter: to analyze a letter written by a
    sergeant before a battle, one needs to know the context in which the
    letter was written, the role of sergeants in the military, and
    knowledge of the war in general (2019).

    There is quite a bit of knowledge that is necessary for anarchists
    to think critically. Existing anarchist theory provides a solid
    foundation: a working knowledge of Bakunin, Kropotkin, Goldman,
    Zerzan, Marx, and others is _ indispensable. With this theory in
    hand, anarchists can learn to identify exploitation, material and
    social inequalities, and the class-based structures inherent to
    capitalism. An understanding of the ideological details of fascism
    and other ideologies opposed to anarchism can help with spotting
    propaganda as well, especially if that propaganda is particularly
    subtle (it doesn’t have to be the Two Minutes Hate to be
    propaganda).

    Familiarity with politics, news, and world events is also
    essential. The world has seen a resurgence of right-wing populism
    recently that is threatening to undermine our collective rights. Any
    good anti-fascist should be able to discuss why it has arisen and how
    to address it. Knowledge about the struggles of our trans, gay,
    disabled, BIPOC, and marginalized brethren is likewise necessary to
    dismantle the barriers preventing us from full equality.

    This list is not complete and is only meant to point critical
    thinkers in the right direction. Remember, knowledge is power, and
    power begins with knowledge.

    3. SEQUENCE

    Willingham’s third step is to identify the order in which
    skills should be learned
    . In most subjects, complex knowledge is
    built on a foundation of more basic information: musicians learn
    scales before they learn to improvise, artists learn to draw basic
    shapes before they draw hands, and math students learn algebra before
    they learn calculus. While this sequence can be flexible (as it
    should be— everyone learns information differently and at different
    rates), here is the sketch of a plan.

    Phase I: Foundations

    This includes learning about the core concepts of anarchism, such
    as anti-authoritarianism, liberty, solidarity, and direct action. One
    should practice spotting power structures in daily life, such as
    police presences and workplace managerial hierarchies. This stage
    should also include practice identifying common statist and
    capitalist arguments.

    Phase II: Critique

    This phase begins applying anarchist ideas from Phase I to
    real-life situations. It includes critiquing capitalism, the state,
    and the media, analyzing the successes and failures of historical
    examples of anarchism, and getting involved in collectives, unions,
    and other groups in the anarchist milieu.

    Phase III: Praxis

    This phase is advanced practice. It includes tackling complex
    debates within anarchism (such as violence vs. pacifism and
    individualism vs. collectivism), critically assessing both anarchist
    and non-anarchist movements, evaluating (and originating) tactics for
    organizing, and creating alternative and anarchist media such as
    zines, papers, and teachins.

    4. REVISIT

    Critical thinking is not something that one learns once and can
    simply use forever. Rather, it takes continual practice to cultivate.
    Willingham stresses that the fourth step is to revisit each
    critical thinking skill
    over time in order to master it. Often
    times the application of these skills will change, as new questions
    and problems arise in which they are put into use. It helps, however,
    to be deliberate about putting these skills into practice.

    Engaging with fellow anarchists and others can help to keep
    critical thinking sharp. Start a reading group to discuss anarchist
    literature or regularly get together with non-anarchists to debate
    the merits of decentralized systems. Join a mutual aid organization
    in order to help others or plan a protest with other activists. The
    opportunities to interact with others are endless.

    Critical thinking skills can be honed individually as well.
    Regularly challenge your own assumptions and thought processses when
    considering important questions or problems. Consider alternate
    scenarios to every solution you find and actively test your ideas in
    the real world. Resist accepting easy answers, and work to apply
    anarchist frameworks to daily life (like using prefigurative
    politics
    to imagine the world as it could be).

    Anarchists often rally a round the slogan “No gods, no masters.”
    While a great phrase, it shouldn’t mean “no thought” as well.
    In fact, anarchism demands more thinking in order to work.
    Willingham may show how critical thinking can be taught, but
    anarchists must take those skills to go forth and build a world
    without domination. In order for this guide to be useful, it should
    be used—so please, go forward and practice these skills (for all of
    our sakes).

    SOURCES

    Bailin, 8., Case, R., Coombs, J. R., & Daniels, L. B. (1999).
    Conceptualizing critical thinking. Journal of Curriculum Studies,
    31(3), 285-302. https://doi.org/10.1080/002202799183133

    Battersby, M. (2016). Js that a fact? (and ed.). Broadview Press.

    Chomsky, N. & Herman, E. S. (2002). Manufacturing consent: The
    political economy of the mass media (and ed.). Random House, Inc.

    Crowder, G. (2005). Anarchism. In E. Craig (ed.), he shorter
    Routledge encyclopedia of philosophy (pp. 14-15). Routledge.
    https://doi.org/10.4324/9780415249126-s003-1

    Ennis, R. (1991). Critical thinking: A streamlined conception.
    Teaching Philosophy, 14/1), 5-24.
    https://doi.org/10.1057/9781137378057_2

    Gelderloos, P. (2004, October). Zhe patriarchal science of
    corporate media. The Anarchist Library.
    https://theanarchistlibrary.org/library/peter-gelderloosthe-patriarchal-science-of-the-corporate-media

    Hasher, L., Goldstein, D., & Toppino, T. (1977). Frequency and
    the conference of referential validity. Journal of Verbal Learning
    and Verbal Behavior, 1&1), 107-112.
    https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-5371(77)80012-1

    Kamm, F. M. (2007). Jntricate ethics: Rights, responsibilities,
    and permissible harm. Oxford University Press.
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:0so/9780195189698.001.0001

    Oswald, M. E. & Grosjean, S. (2004). Confirmation bias. In R.
    F. Pohl (ed.), Cognitive illusions: A handbook on fallacies and
    biases in thinking judgement and Iemory (pp. 79-96). Psychology
    Press.

    Sagan, C. (1979). Broca’s brain: Reflections on the romance of
    science. Ballantine Books.

    Sunstein, C. (2005). Moral heuristics. Behavioral and Brain
    Sciences, 2X4), 531542. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.387941

    Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty:
    Heuristics and biases. Science, 18X4157), 1124-1131.
    https://doi.org/10.21236/ad0767426

    Willingham, D. T. (2019). How to teach critical thinking. NSW
    Department of Education.
    https://education.nsw.gov.au/content/dam/maineducation/teaching-and-learning/education-for-a-changingworld/media/documents/How-to-teach-critical-thinking-Willingham.pdf

    Discussion